We recognise the continuous and deep connection to Country, of Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander peoples as the first peoples of this nation. In this way we respectfully acknowledge the Traditional Custodians of this land, sea, the waters and sky. We pay tribute to the Elders past and present as we also respect the collective ancestry that has brought us all here today.
AUTHOR: CONOR WYNN, PHD
The following article is republished from Sein with permission.
We like to think that big policy decisions are made thoughtfully, informed by data, with careful consideration of the facts and after a deliberate weighing of the options. Not so it seems.
It turns out that the political elite use ‘rules of thumb,’ also known as heuristics, to make the big decisions1. So who are these people, why do they decide that way, and is it OK that they do so?
The elite is a small group of individuals who occupy prominent positions in society, and who have preferential access to resources and an outsized impact on events. They tend to recognise one another, act and think alike. The political elite is a subset, comprised of politicians, senior bureaucrats, and advisers. Politicians in power face a 24/7 news cycle, deal with complex problems and divergent opinions. And though this is what they signed up for, it can lead to the political elite relying on heuristics to cope.
Which heuristics do the political elite use and why?
The literature shows that the political elite are prone to use heuristics such as, being more sensitive to losses than gains, also known as prospect theory, status quo bias, overconfidence – leading to poor decision-making, which when combined with escalation of commitment could help explain why once having made a poor decision, the elite are also prone to doubling down on previous commitments and stereotyping2 to name a few.
There are seven factors that influence the political elite in using heuristics:
- Experience: the greater the experience the more effective use of heuristics3.
- Context: Greater experience in similar contexts appears to allow the more experienced to get to an acceptable outcome more quickly than those with less experience4.
- Complexity: More complex issues are associated with the greater use of heuristics5.
- Urgency: The greater the urgency the more likely the use of heuristics6.
- Self-interest: For example, gaining or remaining in office.
- Ideology: Political ideology such as conservatism or socialism can be used as a heuristic.
- Emotion: For example, the British prime minister Herbert Asquith’s decision to enter World War One was thought to have been influenced by anger and fear7.
The issue is though—is that OK?
Is it good enough, for example, that a decision to go to war is influenced by heuristics, or should we expect a more thoughtful approach before placing troops in harm’s way?
Is it OK to use heuristics for political decision-making?
There are two leading schools of thought around the use of heuristics. The heuristics and bias (H&B) school lead by the Nobel laurate Daniel Kahneman, and the fast and frugal (F&F) school led by Gerd Gigerenzer.
The H&B school argues that there are two styles of decision-making—either quick or deliberate8, however there are a few caveats. These decision-making styles could be thought of as being at opposite ends of a spectrum, and by implication, a blend of these two decision-making styles is likely, as is a sequencing of different decision-making styles, e.g. using heuristics to narrow down the range of choices, then a deliberate approach to choosing between them. But when it comes to political decision-making Kahneman argues that heuristics should not be used9, whereas Gigerenzer allows for their use in political decision-making10. So, which school is right? Unfortunately, it’s not that straightforward.
The problem with political decision-making is that often the answer to a question is unknown in advance, because the problems are complex. The lack of a “correct” answer makes testing alternatives impossible. To make matters worse, decision-making by the political elite make can be motivated by their desire to stay in office11. And so, it’s not possible to test motivated decision-making in an objective sense, because it is subjective—by definition.
Notwithstanding the impossibility of a binary solution to the debate about the use of heuristics in political decision-making, we wanted to learn more. So, we managed to secure rare access to 21 senior ex state politicians, their advisers, and senior former bureaucrats discussing an innovative but politically difficult transport pricing policy proposal.
Transport pricing reform
Road congestion in large cities is a significant issue in Australia. Charges for road use are levied upfront (e.g. vehicle registration tax) and do not reflect actual road usage. An alternate approach to transport network pricing (TNP) would be a user pay system where those who travel during peak times, for greater distances or into highly congested zones would pay more than those who didn’t. And so, there was political risk.
The discussion forum
There were 21 participants, six of whom were current or former senior politicians (from both major political parties), seven current or former senior bureaucrats (i.e. heads of departments), and eight current or former political advisers. Participants were drawn from the two major parties, for balance. And as the participants were no longer in positions of executive power, we hoped to minimise the risk of presentation management overlaying the decision-making process. An extensive briefing document was provided to all participants in advance, including a detailed business case, economic modelling, pricing recommendations, traffic projections and demographic analysis.
What we found — whether to engage and how to engage
We found that the political elite used heuristics in two ways.
First, to decide whether to engage with an issue, using the “wait-and-see” heuristic. And second—having decided whether to engage—how to engage, using political empathy to guide their actions on TNP.
Whether to engage — the “wait and see” heuristic
The primary concern of the elite we observed was not judging the TNP in isolation on its merits, rather whether they ought to engage with the TNP at all. In other words, they didn’t act as judges, considering the facts of each case brought before them. Instead, they behaved like investors, deciding which stock to invest in from a broad market.
So, the first question they asked was – are voters demanding action? They sensed that though there was dissatisfaction about traffic congestion, community sentiment hadn’t crystallised to the point where action was being demanded. The issue wasn’t making headlines, and so, the answer to the first question was “no”, the issue wasn’t urgent and didn’t demand action. This led to the second question; if this issues wasn’t urgent, yet they pressed ahead with implementing the TNP regardless – would there be much resistance?
In a telling comment, a senior politician summed up the situation, based on his experience of trying to change car usage behaviour through pricing signals alone:
“Pricing hasn’t worked. [It] takes a lot of fiscal pain for someone to get out of a car.”
Politicians representing outer suburbs feared community pushback from increased costs which cast a shadow of self-interest over their decision-making.
So, the calculation was – “yes”, there was a risk of strong resistance to those proposals from segments of the community. At this point they thought – the matter isn’t urgent, there’s likely to be pushback and so they asked the third key question – if we impose this policy against the wishes of the electorate…
“are we prepared to spend the political capital needed to overpower that resistance”
This strategy, characterised as the “political strong man” approach had been used in New York, London, and Singapore for example. A former adviser summarised the group’s dilemma at this point in the decision tree.
“It’s [political] suicide analysis, how much damage are you going to do to yourself?”
A former senior bureaucrat summed up the three step decision tree with a pointed question:
“Does any politician think that the big problem is congestion and that the answer is pricing?”
The matter was decided quickly with the answer to this question by a senior former politician:
“No. Not yet.”
How to engage — political empathy
To identify the heuristics voters might use to judge the proposal, the political elite used political empathy – putting themselves in voters’ shoes to identify which heuristics voters would use when judging a policy.
To help them with political empathy they used:
- Stereotyping – the forum established “Cranbourne man” as the typical suburban voter
who is independent, easily upset and whose key concern is access to roads. - Trust – participant’s view was that the greater the trust, the more likely voters would be to support an innovative policy. So, if the government could demonstrate a track record of successful delivery, voters would be more likely to trust them with large issues such as the TNP.
- Incrementalism – the forum thought that if the public became used to new pricing arrangements on electric vehicles, they would be more amenable to the TNP.
- The decoy effect – when an obviously less attractive option is included in a set of alternatives
with the aim of influencing choice towards a recommended option.
And so, the complex issue of whether to engage with the TNP was decided. It was a decision not to engage, or a decision to make no decision. A previously identified12 but not observed heuristic of “wait-and-see” was used to decide the fate of an innovative policy proposal.
So what?
While we identified seven factors that influence the use of heuristics by the political elite, we saw five in play at our forum – experience, context, issue complexity, and urgency. Though our forum members were highly experienced, they were not experts in transport pricing policy and so context was key. The matter was highly complex, and as there was no pressing need to decide, urgency was low. The combination of those factors influenced decision-making style, and caused them to use both styles of thinking, not either.
The politicians we observed made a decision about a decision, which could be considered deliberate decision-making based on elaborate thought, so supporting the F&F school. However, in arriving at that decision they did not consider the details of the extensive briefing materials, rather they used a three-step process to reach an acceptable answer quickly – the hallmark of heuristic decision-making, preferring to “wait-and-see”.
While it looks like an important question – is it OK for heuristics to be used for political decision-making, it turns out that this isn’t a good question after all. This forum showed that both styles of decision-making can be appropriate, rather than one or the other. And while most studies of political decision-making focus on decision-making, few address political non-decision-making. For the first time to our knowledge, we found evidence of the use of heuristics for avoiding a decision, and the shaping of public policy by inaction.
How to avoid indecision or “irrational” decisions from the political elite
When dealing with political elite, there’s a real risk that there either won’t be a decision, or one that “doesn’t make sense”, so what can be done to avoid those poor outcomes?
- Try to re-engage the decision makers on the detail, so forcing deliberate thinking, but there are problems with this approach. The likelihood of success is low – we know the political elite like to use heuristics13. And secondly, as there are some situations where heuristics are preferable to elaborate thinking14, so it’s possible that for instance now is not the right time for the proposal.
- Encourage decision makers to become aware of their biases possibly through leadership development programs. Once again there are problems with this approach. Telling someone important that their decision-making is biased, and they should re-train could be career limiting. And including de-bias training in general leadership development training programs, so that when leaders do emerge into senior roles they rely less on heuristics is a very long-term play. Worse still, there’s evidence that such training programs are either useless15, or can backfire16.
- Use the decoy heuristic by adding an obviously inferior alternative to the one you prefer. While this might have the desired effect it’s ethically questionable. At minimum you could be accused of libertarian paternalism, or at worst manipulative.
- Take a portfolio view of all your policy proposals and put them to the “wait and see” heuristic test to spot which ones might struggle to get leadership engagement. This has legs. It recognises that the political elite use heuristics rather than pushes back against it, and so is a pragmatic choice. It provides feedback on which of your proposals is likely to be successful and which could end up in deep freeze. Armed with that information, you could re-allocate your resources to those proposals with greater chances of success, becoming more effective in the process. And, in looking at those proposals that didn’t pass the “wait-and see” test, you might discover which conditions need to change or be changed for them to pass the test.
In summary, there’s evidence that heuristics have their place in decision-making, and that a blend of deliberate thinking and heuristics is effective.
But the issue is not the theoretical one of whether important people ought not use heuristics, it’s the pragmatic one of how to cope with the fact that they do. The “wait-and-see” heuristic is alive and well among the political elite, understanding how to deal with it is key.
- Stolwijk, S., & Vis, B. (2020). Politicians, the Representativeness Heuristic and Decision-Making Biases. Political Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-020-09594-6
- Bordalo, P., Coffman, K., Gennaioli, N., & Shleifer, A. (2016). Stereotypes. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 131(4), 1753-1794. https://doi.org/10.1093/qje/qjw029
- Hafner-Burton, E. M., Hughes, D. A., & Victor, D. G. (2013). The Cognitive Revolution and the Political Psychology of Elite Decision Making. Perspectives on Politics, 11(2), 368-386. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1537592713001084
- Klein, G. (2008). Naturalistic decision making. Human Factors, 50(3), 456-460.
- MacGillivray, B. H. (2014). Fast and frugal crisis management: An analysis of rule-based judgment and choice during water contamination events. Journal of Business Research, 67(8), 1717-1724. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.02.018
- Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (1999). Dual-process theories in social psychology. Guilford Press.
- Young, J. W. (2018). Emotions and the British Government’s Decision for War in 1914. Diplomacy & Statecraft, 29(4), 543-564. https://doi.org/10.1080/09592296.2018.1528778
- Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Macmillan.
- Kahneman, D., & Klein, G. (2009). Conditions for intuitive expertise: a failure to disagree. American Psychologist, 64(6), 515.
- Gigerenzer, G. (2008). Why heuristics work. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(1), 20-29.
- Bowler, S., Donovan, T., & Karp, J. A. (2006). Why politicians like electoral institutions: Self- interest, values, or ideology? The Journal of Politics, 68(2), 434-446.
- Walgrave, S., & Dejaeghere, Y. (2017). Surviving information overload: How elite politicians select information. Governance, 30(2), 229-244.
- Stolwijk, S., & Vis, B. (2020). Politicians, the Representativeness Heuristic and Decision-Making Biases. Political Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-020-09594-6
- Gigerenzer, G., & Goldstein, D. G. (2011). The recognition heuristic: A decade of research. Judgment and Decision Making, 6(1), 100-121.
- Noon, M. (2018). Pointless diversity training: Unconscious bias, new racism and agency. Work, employment and society, 32(1), 198-209.
- Atewologun, D., Cornish, T., & Tresh, F. (2018). Unconscious bias training: An assessment of the evidence for effectiveness. Equality and Human Rights Commission Research Report Series.
This article is based on an article published in the Australian Journal of Public Administration, the peer reviewed journal of the Institute of Public Administration Australia.